Skip to main content

Effectiveness of the serious game ‘You & I’ in changing mentalizing abilities of adults with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities: a parallel superiority randomized controlled trial

Abstract

Background

Persons with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities generally show dysfunctions in mentalization and stress regulation, resulting in problematic social relationships and personal distress. Intervention programs may improve mentalizing abilities. The aim of this study is to examine the effectiveness of the serious game ‘You & I’ in changing mentalizing abilities and stress regulation in adults with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities.

Methods

A two-arm, parallel, superiority randomized controlled trial will be used with 172 adults with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities. Participants will be randomly assigned to either the experimental group to play the serious game ‘You & I’ or a waitlist control group. Participants will be assessed at baseline, post intervention (5 weeks after baseline), and follow-up (6–8 weeks after post intervention). They also will fill in questionnaires for personal factors, personal development, personal well-being, social validity, autism spectrum quotient (demographic variables), mentalizing abilities (primary outcome measure), and stress regulation (secondary outcome measure).

Discussion

The serious game ‘You & I’ aims to improve mentalizing abilities in adults with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities, which is expected to lead to improved regulation of stress in social relationships. The study’s unique feature is the use of a serious game to improve mentalizing abilities. If the intervention is effective, the serious game can be implemented on a broad scale in Dutch care organizations for people with intellectual disabilities as an effective preventive tool to improve mentalizing abilities.

Trial registration

Netherlands Trial Register, NTR7418. Registered on 2 August 2018.

Peer Review reports

Background

“Seeing oneself from the outside and others from the inside” [1]. This phrase is commonly used to describe mentalization, a concept that has been gaining attention since Fonagy reintroduced it in the early 1990s. Mentalization refers to “the mental process by which an individual implicitly and explicitly interprets the actions of himself or herself and others as meaningful on the basis of intentional mental states such as personal desires, needs, feelings, beliefs, and reasons” [2]. The capacity for mentalization is acquired in early childhood and is related to development in the domains of cognition, language, and social–emotional functioning [3]. Developmental delays in these domains in persons with mild to borderline intellectual disability (MBID) might be linked to limited capacity to mentalize [3]. It is important to examine whether interventions can help improve the mentalizing capacity of people with MBID.

Internal mental states drive how humans respond and act in certain situations. Mentalization involves recognition of mental states of the self and others, bringing beliefs, desires, intentions, goals, and emotions into awareness [4, 5]. Being aware of these mental states in the self and others facilitates coping, perceiving others as potential sources of support, and social alignment. Persons with MBID have a limited capacity to mentalize [3]. Predicting behavior of others and anticipating is therefore challenging [6,7,8]. Dysfunctions in mentalizing abilities may explain problems in social functioning among persons with MBID, such as destructive social interactions and social exploitation [9, 10]. Furthermore, because of limited social information processing as an executive function, people with MBID perceive more negative information, along with more difficulty remembering and processing information, limited working memory, and fewer problem-solving skills. The resulting stress makes learning mentalizing abilities more difficult [11, 12].

Within the construct of mentalization, three dimensions can be identified [13]. The first dimension is related to two modes of functioning, namely implicit and explicit functioning. Implicit mentalization refers to the unconscious, procedural, or automatic system of understanding others. Explicit mentalization involves a deliberate and conscious focus on mental content [14]. The second dimension is related to two objects, specifically the self and others. An individual can mentalize not only one’s own mental states but also the mental states of others; moreover, a person can mentalize one’s own relationships with other persons and, correspondingly, other persons’ relationships with one another [15]. The third dimension of the mentalization concept relates to its cognitive and affective aspects. Mental states in oneself and others can be cognitively focused as well as affectively laden to varying degrees [13].

According to Allen et al. [14] and Fonagy [16], humans first acquire the capacity for mentalization in the context of early attachment relationships. More specifically, the child develops mentalizing abilities within a secure attachment relationship, when the caregiver accurately and contingently mirrors the internal states of the child [13, 17]. The mirroring also needs to be marked. The caregiver contingently mirrors the internal state of the child [18]. This mirroring process enables the child to understand their own mental states and develop a stable sense of identity and, in turn, regulate their affect and distress [13, 18]. By also naming the mental state of the child, the child learns to recognize and name their mental state and those of others, which will improve their mentalizing abilities [19, 20].

Mentalization is not a static capacity but a dynamic, multifaceted ability whose development can be influenced by external factors such as stress [21, 22]. As Allen [15] points out: “Stress is the enemy of mentalization”. When high levels of stress are experienced, adequate mentalization becomes more difficult [23, 24]. To be more precise, the capacity to understand someone else’s mental states may be reduced, distorted, and less flexible when high levels of stress are experienced [22]. On the other hand, the ability to mentalize contributes to resilience against stress. In conclusion, support for mentalization may improve functioning even under circumstances that otherwise would have been experienced as stressful.

Awareness and understanding of mental states can be developed gradually by explicitly educating someone about mental states [25]. Empirical evidence suggests that mentalizing abilities can be improved with treatment programs such as mentalization-based therapy (MBT) [2]. In MBT, a therapist asks questions to strengthen the patient’s mentalizing abilities, such as ‘How do you think and feel about yourself and others?’, ‘Does this influence your behavior?’, ‘Can miscommunications in behavior and feelings lead to difficulties?’, and ‘How can you prevent and resolve those miscommunications?’ [16]. Randomized trials have shown that mentalization-based therapies improve interpersonal and social functioning [26, 27]. So far, treatment programs such as mentalization-based therapy have mainly focused on persons with personality disorders, while methods to stimulate the acquisition of mentalizing abilities for persons with MBID are still lacking. Moreover, current treatments for improving mentalizing abilities are time-consuming (treatment duration varying between 4 and 18 months), require supervision from qualified mental health professionals, and, consequently, are quite costly [28,29,30].

A promising and innovative method to improve mentalizing abilities of people with MBID is serious gaming. Serious games are computer applications that combine serious aspects, such as learning, with playful gaming elements [31]. Serious games have become increasingly popular over the years and results on their effectiveness are promising [32, 33]. Studies have also shown that serious games can be deployed successfully in the care for persons with intellectual disabilities when it comes to learning new skills and the development of abstract concepts [34]. Furthermore, MBT key elements can be realized in the serious game: a voice-over can, just like a therapist, instruct people with MBID indirectly (i.e. through interaction with an identifiable character) to recognize and name their mental state and those of others, which will improve their mentalizing abilities. Serious games can support learning which is particularly helpful for people with MBID, who learn by making abstract concepts concrete and receiving and processing new information step by step [3]. Moreover, serious games can be deployed at low cost and provide a unique learning environment wherein persons are allowed to practice new skills in a setting that is (un)likely to be realized in their daily lives [35, 36].

Trial objective

The serious game ‘You & I’ (in Dutch: ‘Jij & Ik’) is a computer game developed with and for adults with MBID. The aim of the game is to improve mentalizing abilities, and stress regulation, focusing specifically on the three aforementioned dimensions of mentalization. In this study, the effectiveness of the serious game will be investigated, answering the following research questions: does the serious game ‘You & I’ improve mentalizing abilities in people with MBID; and does the serious game ‘You & I’ improve regulation of stress in adults with MBID?

Hypotheses

The serious game ‘You & I’ is hypothesized to have a positive effect on mentalizing abilities, including the regulation of stress, in adults with MBID. The primary hypothesis is that being randomly assigned to the experimental group that plays the serious game will be associated with an improvement of the mentalizing abilities in adults with MBID as compared to being assigned to a waitlist control group. The secondary hypothesis is that being randomly assigned to the experimental group playing the serious game will be associated with an improvement of stress regulation in adults with MBID.

Methods

Effectiveness will be examined using a parallel superiority randomized controlled trial (RCT) with a baseline, a post test after 4 weeks, and a follow-up assessment after 6–8 weeks (see Fig. 1). The RCT includes two groups: an experimental group who play the serious game ‘You & I’; and a control group who will be placed on a waitlist. The method of this study is reported according to the Standard Protocol Items: Recommendations for Interventional Trails (see Additional file 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the study timeline

Study population

A total of 172 adults with MBID, aged 18 years or older, will be recruited from the population of four Dutch care organizations that are specialized in disability care (Bartiméus, Ons Tweede Thuis, Cordaan, and ASVZ). Furthermore, the possibility to participate will be mentioned on websites (e.g. www.socialerelatiesenict.nl), on social media, and at various meetings. A diagnosis of MBID (IQ ranging between 50 and 85) needs to be reported, for instance, by the care organizations where the participant receives care. Inclusion criteria are having basic computer operation skills and having access to a computer with Internet connectivity. Adults need to give written consent for participation in the study and, if necessary, also their legal representative. Excluded from participation in the study are adults who are deaf and/or blind or adults who have serious mobility impairments for whom computer operation is not possible without aids.

Sample size calculation

Linear mixed effect modeling with two conditions and three repeated measures will be conducted to analyze the effects on mentalizing abilities and stress regulation. The sample size is estimated based on previous studies measuring the mentalizing ability of perspective-taking using the Perspective Taking (PT) subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and stress regulation using the Lifestress Inventory (LI) among people with intellectual disabilities. With the means used from both measures (for means, see [37, 38]), a desired power of 0.90, and α = 0.05, it is estimated that around 144 participants are needed, as calculated in GLIMMPSE [39]. Because there are three assessments, dropout of 20% will be taken into account. Thus, 172 participants will be recruited and randomized into two groups of approximately 86 participants in each group.

Study procedure and randomization

Individuals who sign up for the research both via the care organization as well as via the Internet or any other route and who meet the inclusion criteria will receive an information brochure. Persons who want to participate in the study are asked to sign the consent form and return it to the researcher. In case of legal incapacitation, the legal representative of the participant is asked to sign and return a consent form on behalf of the participant.

Data collection for each participant takes 12–14 weeks. Participants will be assessed at baseline (T0), post intervention (T1, 5 weeks after baseline), and follow up (T2, 6–8 weeks after T1). During all three assessments, participants fill out a set of digital questionnaires (for all measurements, see Measures). A software program (e.g. Qualtrics or Survalyzer) will be used to gather data through the digital questionnaires. Completing the questionnaires will take up to 90 min per assessment. During all assessments, an independent researcher is present to assist the participants with completing the questionnaire at home or in their care home. The researchers will follow a standard protocol on how to assist the participants.

After informed consent and baseline assessment, blind for intervention, participants will be individually randomized into two groups using stratified randomization in combination with block randomization with varying block sizes of 4 and 6. To balance contextual factors, randomization will be stratified with regard to care organization. An independent researcher will produce the allocation schedule using a computerized random number generator and afterward conceal the schedule for the researchers. Blinding is only possible for the baseline assessment and, after that, both participants and the researchers will know to which group participants have been assigned.

After randomization, participants within the experimental group will be offered the serious game ‘You & I’, while participants within the control group will be placed on a waitlist. Participants from the experimental group will be asked to play the serious game on their own computer device at home or on a computer device of their care home. They have to complete eight gaming levels within 4 weeks, playing the game twice a week. To remind the participants to play the game, they will receive an impersonal email or text message on their phone twice a week asking them whether they have already played the game. Anonymous digital game statistics will measure the compliance of the participants (how often the computer game has been completed). After 4 weeks, the post-intervention assessment is administered, and 6–8 weeks later the participants complete the follow-up assessment. Participants from the control group can play the serious game after they have completed the follow-up assessment.

Intervention

The intervention is a serious game called ‘You & I’ that focuses on the improvement of mentalizing abilities, including the regulation of stress. The second and last authors in collaboration with adults with MBID and healthcare professionals developed the serious game. The serious game ‘You & I’ is based on attachment theory [40], the practice-oriented book Mentalizing in Clinical Practice by Allen et al. [14] and the practice-oriented book Mentalization Can Be Learned (in Dutch: Mentaliseren kan je leren) by Dekker-van der Sande and Sterkenburg [3]. The participant with MBID can play the game independently on a tablet or computer.

The serious game revolves around a main character called Mo, who the player follows throughout the game by watching videos. In the first level, the player learns that Mo is sad because he misses his friend Emily, who moved to the United States. He decides to visit her and travel to the United States. The player will follow Mo on his adventure, while he leaves his house, takes the bus and the airplane, and finds his way through a foreign country to finally be able to visit Emily.

The game consists of eight gaming levels, which will take about 30–45 min to complete. The participant is asked to play the game twice a week, completing one level every time. Each level has the same structure consisting of eight different elements. That is, videos following Mo’s journey, multiple-choice questions, an emotion picture game, a stress measurer, and a game about stress. The gaming levels cover different domains of mentalization, as described by Choi-Kain and Gunderson [13]. Table 1 presents an overview of the themes and the domains of mentalization that are covered in each particular level. The first six gaming levels each cover a different domain of mentalization and levels seven and eight are so-called ‘booster levels’, implementing and repeating all domains of mentalization. By integrating the different domains of mentalization in the levels of the serious game, the player will improve their mentalizing abilities and learn how to cope with stress better.

Table 1 Overview of themes and domains of mentalization for each level of the serious game ‘You & I’

Measures

All data will be collected through computerized assessments at baseline, post intervention, and follow-up assessment. Participants can fill out the digital questionnaires at home or in their care home. When needed, participants will receive support from an independent researcher, who will be present during all assessments and who will follow a standard protocol on how to assist the participants. Figure 2 provides an overview of the measures and time of assessment.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Schedule of enrollment, allocation, intervention, and assessments

Demographic variables

Minimal dataset (MDS; T0)

To measure demographic variables, the minimal dataset (MDS) ‘Basic MDS’ and ‘Basic MDS for adults with an intellectual disability’ will be used, including the Personal Wellbeing Index—Intellectual Disability (PWI-ID) [41, 42]. The MDS is a set of questions on demographic variables for everyone who collects data of persons with intellectual disabilities. The MDS focuses on questions in the following domains: personal factors, personal development, and personal well-being. The questionnaire consists of 32 items, measuring, for example, gender, age, and intellectual functioning.

Social Validity Scale (SVS; T0 and T1)

The SVS [43] is a questionnaire consisting of 15 questions measured on a 5-point Likert scale to assess the desirability, applicability, clarity, and efficiency of the intervention procedure. In this study, the scale will be used as described by Janssen et al. [44] and Jonker et al. [45]. During baseline assessment, participants answer questions concerning their expectations of the serious game ‘You & I’ at the post-intervention assessment questions concerning their experiences with playing the game.

Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ-10; T0)

The AQ-10 [46] measures the degree to which adults with average intelligence exhibit autistic traits. The self-report questionnaire consists of 10 items measured on a 4-point Likert scale with scores ranging from definitely agree (1) to definitely disagree (4). The first items of the measure are ‘I often notice small sounds when others do not’ and ‘I usually concentrate more on the whole picture, rather than the small details’. Within a normal developing population, the AQ-10 performs well at discriminating between individuals with and without a clinical diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder [47]. The AQ-10 has not yet been used for adults with intellectual disabilities. Therefore, the items were adapted for persons with MBID. The adaptations were made by three authors (SvW, MW, PSS) and checked by collaborating researchers with MBID to align with our target group.

Mentalization

The primary outcome measure of this study is mentalizing abilities. Several questionnaires will be used to measure mentalization, each measuring a different component of mentalization (i.e. reflective functioning, perspective-taking, emotion recognition, and the attribution of mental states). No specific effect is expected, and therefore the aggregate of the measures is tested to investigate what components are affected by the intervention.

The Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (RFQ; T0, T1 and T2)

The RFQ [48] is a brief self-report screening measure of mentalizing abilities. It consists of eight items measured on a 7-point Likert scale with scores ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). The first three items are ‘People’s thoughts are a mystery to me’, ‘I don’t always know why I do what I do’, and ‘When I get angry, I say things without really knowing why I am saying them’. Psychometric properties are good in a normal developing population and in patients with personality disorders. For the purpose of this study, the measure was adapted for adults with intellectual disabilities by removing unnecessary wording and simplifying concepts. The adaptations were made by the first three authors and checked by collaborating researchers with MBID. Moreover, eight experimental items from the RFQ-54 were added to the questionnaire. The instrument was translated into Dutch by the second author. Then, it was translated back to English by the last author. Where necessary, adjustments were made. Any ambiguity was discussed in mail conversation with the developers of the instrument. Therefore, existing psychometric property data did not apply.

Radboud Faces Database (RaFD; T0, T1 and T2)

The RaFD [49] is a set of pictures depicting different emotional expressions and is used to assess emotion recognition as a part of mentalization. Participants have to view color photographs of unfamiliar faces portraying 10 different Caucasian and Moroccan adults each displaying five emotions (anger, fear, happiness, sadness, and neutral). A selection of 50 photographs has been made based on the percentage of agreement on emotion categorization, mean intensity, mean clarity, mean genuineness of the emotion, and mean valence of the photograph [49]. The pictures include averted gaze orientations (left and right) as well as direct gaze orientations (frontal). Participants have to indicate for each photograph which one of five emotions the adult depicts. The RaFD has good psychometric qualities in a normal developing population, with an average expression agreement between chosen and targeted emotions of 82% (median 88%, SD = 19%) [49].

Perspective Taking (PT) subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; T0, T1 and T2)

The IRI [37] is a multidimensional tool measuring interpersonal reactivity. The self-report questionnaire consists of 28 items measured on a 5-point Likert scale with scores ranging from does not describe me well (1) to describes me very well (5). The measure has four subscales, each made up of seven different items. In this study, only the PT subscale will be used. The PT subscale measures the tendency to take the psychological point of view of others. The first three items are ‘I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the other person’s point of view’, ‘I try to look at everybody’s side of a disagreement before I make a decision’, and ‘I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how things look from their perspective’. The reliability of the subscale is adequate, with Cronbach’s α = 0.73 [50]. A modification of the subscale has previously been used in research on adults with moderate or mild intellectual disabilities, which also indicated adequate reliability for this population with Cronbach’s α = 0.71 [50].

Frith–Happé Animations Test (T0, T1 and T2)

The Frith–Happé Animations Test is added because this measure has been previously used with children with intellectual disabilities [51]. It is a nonverbal task to measure mentalizing abilities and therefore is a good addition to the other verbal questionnaires. The Frith–Happé Animations Test [51] consists of a series of computer-presented animations, each lasting 34–45 s. All animations feature one large red and one small blue triangle moving around the screen. There are three types of animations. First, Theory of Mind (ToM) animations in which it is suggested that the triangle anticipates or manipulates the ‘mental state’ of the other. Second, goal-direct action (GD) animations in which the interaction between the triangles can be described in terms of behavioral interaction. Third, random (Rd) animations in which the triangles purposelessly move around without reference to interactions, goals, or intentions.

After each animation, participants were asked ‘What was happening in the animation?’ Verbal descriptions are recorded and scored for complexity of mental state terms used (i.e. intentionality; 0–3) and accuracy of the answer given (i.e. appropriateness; 0–2). Participants are presented with two practice animations (GD and ToM) to ensure they understand the task.

Stress regulation

The secondary outcome measure of this study is stress regulation.

Lifestress Inventory (LI; T0, T1 and T2)

The LI [52] is a 30-item self-report questionnaire which can be used to measure general worry, negative interpersonal interactions, and competency concerns. Participants are first asked to indicate whether they have experienced a stressor. If they do not, participants move on to the next item. If they do, they select one of four answers to indicate the impact of the stressor, ranging from no stress (1) to a great deal of stress (5). The first three items are ‘Do people treat you as though you are different?’, ‘Have you been getting on with your partner/girlfriend/boyfriend?’, and ‘Have you heard people you know arguing?’ The LI is reliable for administration of people with ID, with Cronbach’s α = 0.85 [52].

Perceived self-efficacy scale (stress; T0, T1 and T2)

This is a short nine-item questionnaire which can be used to measure perceived self-efficacy regarding stress regulation. The questionnaire is designed by the researchers of this study using Bandura’s guide for constructing self-efficacy scales [53] and is specifically focused on the skills that have been learned in the serious game ‘You & I’. Self-efficacy is concerned with people’s expectations of executing a particular skill, in this case stress regulation. We expect that if people are aware of actions that have the effect of regulating stress, this will lead to better stress regulation. Participants are asked on a scale from 0 (not at all sure) to 10 (very sure) how certain they are about how they can know, feel, and cope with stress. The first three items are ‘Feel in my body when I have stress’, ‘Deal with stress well’, and ‘Know that I have stress’.

Data analysis

All statistical analysis will be conducted using SPSS version 24.0. Descriptive statistics will give insight into the characteristics of the participants. Before analyzing, outliers will be checked and, if necessary, winsorized and partial intention-to-treat analysis will be performed. Demographic variables are used to test for differences in baseline characteristics between the experimental and control groups, and are added as covariates if differences are found. For social validity, average item scores are reported.

Primary and secondary outcome measures of the study (i.e. mentalizing abilities and stress regulation) will be assessed using linear mixed-effects modeling. With Subject at the highest level and Group, Time, and the Group × Time interaction entered as fixed effects, a mixed model fits into SPSS. Furthermore, compliance will be controlled for through anonymous digital game statistics (how often the computer game is completed) and care organization will be the stratifying variable.

Data management and monitoring

Data will be collected using online survey software. Computerized data will be stored on a secured server of Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. The participant’s privacy is guaranteed by assigning a unique identification number to every participant. Data will be processed using these identification numbers. All researchers who will work with the research data will sign a nondisclosure agreement, stating that they will not share personal details of participants with a third party. The handling of the data will comply with the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR). A data management plan was submitted and accepted by the funding organization of the study (ZonMw; project number 845004004). This study is also embedded in the Amsterdam Public Health (APH) Research Institute. The quality committee of APH offers a handbook to safeguard the quality of the research and performs random audits.

Ethical considerations

Ethical approval was given by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Amsterdam location VUmc, the Netherlands (METc VUmc 2018.007, NL60353.029.17) and the Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam (VCWE-2017-171). Potential future changes to the study will be proposed to the Medical Ethics Committee as amendments, and will be described and discussed in publications of this study hereafter.

Discussion

This article describes the study protocol of a parallel superiority randomized controlled trial for examining the effectiveness of the serious game ‘You & I’ in improving mentalizing abilities, including the regulation of stress, in adults with MBID. Participants in the experimental condition are offered to play ‘You & I’ for a duration of 4 weeks, while participants in the control condition are placed on a waitlist. It is hypothesized that participants in the experimental condition will show significant improvement in mentalizing abilities compared to participants from the waitlist control condition, as the experimental group is offered to play a serious game that is tailored for its specific purpose.

A unique feature of this study is the use of a serious game for the improvement of mentalizing abilities. This study is among the first to use a serious game specifically targeted at mentalizing abilities, particularly in adults with MBID. Serious games are promising within healthcare for persons with intellectual disabilities because their entertaining nature and familiarity to the target population offers new, alternative modalities for intervention that can improve engagement with intervention in high-risk groups. Serious games require few costs to deploy on a large scale, making these potentially cost-effective even at low levels of effectiveness. The threshold is low as it is home-based and requires only the use of a computer and Internet access [35, 54], leading to increased accessibility over face-to-face interventions such as mentalization-based therapy. If this study shows that the serious game has a positive effect on the abilities of the participants, serious games have the potential to become a standard service in healthcare for persons with MBID.

In addition to an innovative psychological intervention, the study incorporates a large group of participants from a less frequently studied population. Only little research has been carried out on adults with MBID, while they actually represent the largest group within the population of persons with intellectual disabilities [55]. A randomized controlled trial with a large sample size is especially exceptional in this field of research [56]. In this study, participants are recruited in close collaboration with four Dutch care organizations. Thus, if the serious game ‘You & I’ appears to be efficacious, the game can be instantaneously implemented as a component of already offered services for adults with MBID.

Participants who will be included in this study are recruited from a wide range of the population of adults with MBID. Persons excluded from this study (i.e. persons who are deaf and/or blind, persons who cannot operate a computer, persons younger than age 18 years) can be used to gain insight into the accessibility of this intervention for this population. Hence, participants may be diagnosed with other comorbid disorders such as autism or borderline personality disorder, have different ethnic backgrounds, come from different socioeconomic situations, and may have varying ages. This wide inclusion of participants has both advantages and disadvantages. The advantage is that external validity becomes a strong aspect of this study. The results will be easily generalizable to the population with MBID, as we expect to have a large range of participants from persons needing a lot to persons needing very little support from care organizations. Nevertheless, the disadvantage of few exclusion criteria is that the effects of this study may be smaller than expected, as the sample will be more heterogeneous.

Another limitation of this study is the use of measures that have not been specifically validated for the targeted population. As research on adults with MBID is scarce, the range of measurement instruments that are validated for this population is also more limited, especially for novel constructs such as mentalizing abilities. Therefore it is possible that the questionnaires will be influenced by language abilities of the participants [57]. To overcome this limitation, for both the primary outcome as well as the secondary outcome, both a verbal and a nonverbal measure have been selected, decreasing the chance of the results being confounded by the participants’ vocabulary competence. Furthermore, some questionnaires have been adapted to make them more suitable for the population, minimizing the possible effect of language on the results. Other researchers might use the results on the measures in future research so that measures for this population can be further developed.

In conclusion, the present study is expected to provide valuable insight into the effectiveness of the serious game ‘You & I’ for adults with MBID. If the intervention is effective, the serious game can be readily implemented on a broad scale in the care organizations for people with intellectual disabilities, thanks to the low cost of deployment. This may mean that fewer persons with MBID will suffer from problems related to mentalization deficits, such as social problems. Possibly, this may lead to less social problems and more social inclusion of persons with MBID.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset generated and/or analyzed during the current study will be available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

MBID:

Mild to borderline intellectual disabilities

MBT:

Mentalization-based treatment

References

  1. Fonagy P, Bateman AW. Progress in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;188:1–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Bateman AW, Fonagy P. Mentalization-based treatment of BPD. J Personal Disord. 2004;18:36–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Dekker-van der Sande F, Sterkenburg P. Mentalization can be learned. Doorn: Bartiméus; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Feenstra D, Bales D. Mentalization-based treatment voor adolescenten. Kinder-Jeugdpsychoterapie. 2015;42:5–20.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Bateman A, Campbell C, Luyten P, Fonagy P. A mentalization-based approach to common factors in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Curr Opin Psychol. 2018;21:44–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Baglio G, Blasi V, Intra FS, Castelli I, Massaro D, Baglio F, et al. Social competence in children with borderline intellectual functioning: delayed development of theory of mind across all complexity levels. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Happé FGE. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24:129–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Yirmiya N, Erel O, Shaked M, Solomonica-levi D. Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychol Bull. 1998;124:283–307.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Bigby C. Known well by no-one: trends in the informal social networks of middle-aged and older people with intellectual disability five years after moving to the community. J Intellect Develop Disabil. 2008;33:148–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. McClure KS, Halpern J, Wolper PA, Donahue JJ. Emotion regulation and intellectual disability. J Dev Disabil. 2014;15:38–44.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Van Nieuwenhuijzen M. Sociale informatieverwerking van kinderen met lichte verstandelijke beperkingen. Kind en Adolesc. 2007;28:97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Janssen C, Schuengel C. Gehechtheid, stress, gedragsproblemen en psychopathologie bij mensen met een lichte verstandelijke beperking: aanzetten voor interventie. In: Didden R, editor. In perspectief. Gedragsproblemen, psychiatrische stoornissen en lichte verstandelijke beperking. Houten: Bohn Stafleu van Loghum; 2006. p. 67–84.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Choi-Kain LW, Gunderson JG. Mentalization: ontogeny, assessment, and application in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1127–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Allen JG, Fonagy P, Bateman AW. Mentalizing in clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Allen JG. Mentalizing. Bull Menn Clin. 2003;67:91–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Fonagy P. Mentalization-Based Treatment (MBT). In: Zeigler-Hill V., Shackelford T. (eds) Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. Cham: Springer; 2017. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_912-1

    Google Scholar 

  17. Bateman AW, Ryle A, Fonagy P, Kerr IB. Psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder: mentalization based therapy and cognitive analytic therapy compared. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007;19:51–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist E, Target M. Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Verheugt-Pleiter JE, Schmeets MGJ, Zevalkink J. Mentaliseren in de kindertherapie. Koninklijke Van Gorcum: Assen; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Midgley N, Ensink K, Lindqvist K, Malberg N, Muller N. Mentalization-based treatment for children: a time-limited approach. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2017.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  21. Bateman AW, Fonagy P. Handbook of mentalizing in mental health practice. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Nolte T, Bolling DZ, Hudac CM, Fonagy P, Mayes L, Pelphrey KA. Brain mechanisms underlying the impact of attachment-related stress on social cognition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Fonagy P, Bateman A. The development of borderline personality disorder—a mentalizing model. J Personal Disord. 2008;22:4–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Fonagy P, Luyten P. A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:1355–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Blakemore S-J, Frith U. The learning brain: lessons for education. Dev Sci. 2005;6:459–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Bateman A, Fonagy P. Randomized controlled trial of outpatient mentalization-based treatment versus structured clinical management for borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:1355–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Jørgensen CR, Freund C, Bøye R, Jordet H, Andersen D, Kjølbye M. Outcome of mentalization-based and supportive psychotherapy in patients with borderline personality disorder: a randomized trial. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013;127:305–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Allen JG, Fonagy P. Handbook of mentalization-based treatment. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 2006.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  29. Bales D, van Beek N, Smits M, Willemsen S, Busschbach JJV, Verheul R, et al. Treatment outcome of 18-month, day hospital mentalization-based treatment (MBT) in patients with severe borderline personality disorder in the Netherlands. J Personal Disord. 2012;26:568–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Laurenssen EMP, Eeren HV, Kikkert MJ, Peen J, Westra D, Dekker JJM, et al. The burden of disease in patients eligible for mentalization-based treatment (MBT): quality of life and costs. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2016;14:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Alvarez J, Damien D. An introduction to serious game: definitions and concepts. In: Proceedings of the Serious Games & Simulation Workshop. Paris; 2011. p. 10–5.

  32. Connolly TM, Boyle EA, MacArthur E, Hainey T, Boyle JM. A systematic literature review of empirical evidence on computer games and serious games. Comput Educ. 2012;59:661–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Wouters P, Van der Spek ED, Van Oostendorp H. Current practices in serious game research: a review from a learning outcomes perspective. In: Connolly TM, Stansfield M, Boyle L, editors. Games-based learning advancements for multi-sensory human computer interfaces: techniques and effective practices. Hershey: IGI Global; 2009. p. 232–50.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  34. Den Brok WLJE, Sterkenburg PS. Self-controlled technologies to support skill attainment in persons with an autism spectrum disorder and/or an intellectual disability: a systematic literature review. Disabil Rehabil Assist Technol. 2015;10:1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Ypsilanti A, Vivas AB, Räisänen T, Viitala M, Ijäs T, Ropes D. Are serious video games something more than a game? A review on the effectiveness of serious games to facilitate intergenerational learning. Educ Inf Technol. 2014;19:515–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Sterkenburg PS, Vacaru VS. The effectiveness of a serious game to enhance empathy for care workers for people with disabilities: a parallel randomized controlled trial. Disabil Health J. 2018;11:576–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Cat Sel Doc Psychol. 1980;10:1–18.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Fogarty GJ, Bramston P. Validation of the Lifestress Inventory for People with Mild Intellectual Handicap. Res Dev Disabil. 1997;18(6):435–56.

  39. Kreidler SM, Muller KE, Grunwald GK, Ringham BM, Coker-Dukowithz ZT, Sakhadeo UR, et al. GLIMMPSE: online power computation for linear models with and without a baseline covariate. J Stat Softw. 2013;54:380–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. Am J Orthop. 1982;52:664–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Cummins RA, Lau ALD. Personal Wellbeing Index—School Children (PWI-SC) (Cantonese). 3rd ed. Victoria; 2005.

  42. Cummins RA, Lau ALD. Personal Wellbeing Index: Pre-school. Burwood: Deakin University; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Seys DM. Kwaliteit van zorg: zorg voor kwaliteit. Nijmegen: Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Janssen MJ, Riksen-Walraven JM, Van Dijk JPM. Enhancing the quality of interaction between deafblind children and their educators. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2002;14:87–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Jonker D, Sterkenburg PS, Van Rensburg E. Caregiver-mediated therapy for an adult with visual and intellectual impairment suffering from separation anxiety. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;47:1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Allison C, Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S. Toward brief “red flags” for autism screening: the short Autism Spectrum Quotient and the short Quantitative Checklist in 1,000 cases and 3,000 controls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:202–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Booth T, Murray AL, McKenzie K, Kuenssberg R, O’Donnell M, Burnett H. Brief report: An evaluation of the AQ-10 as a brief screening instrument for ASD in adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:2997–3000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Moulton-perkins A, Lee Y, Warren F, Howard S, et al. Development and validation of a self-report measure of mentalizing: the Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. PLoS One. 2016;11:1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Langner O, Dotsch R, Bijlstra G, Wigboldus DHJ, Hawk ST, Van Knippenberg A. Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cogn Emot. 2010;24:1377–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. De Corte K, Buysse A, Verhofstadt L, Roeyers H, Ponnet K, Davis MH. Measuring empathic tendencies: reliability and validity of the Dutch version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Psychol Belg. 2014;47:235–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Michie AM, Lindsay WR. A treatment component designed to enhance empathy in sex offenders with an intellectual disability. Br J Forensic Pract. 2012;14:40–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Abell F, Happé F, Frith U. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn Dev. 2000;15:1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Bandura A. Guidelines for constructing self-efficacy scales. In: Pajares F, Urdan T, editors. Self-efficacy, beliefs of adolescents. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing; 2006. p. 307–37.

  54. De Freitas S, Oliver M. How can exploratory learning with games and simulations within the curriculum be most effectively evaluated? Comput Educ. 2006;46:249–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Woittiez I, Putman L, Eggink E, Ras M, Sterkenburg PS. Zorg Beter Begrepen. Den Haag: Sociaal en Cultureel Planbureau; 2014. p. 128.

  56. Mulhall P, Taggart L, Coates V, McAloon T, Hassiotis A. A systematic review of the methodological and practical challenges of undertaking randomised-controlled trials with cognitive disability populations. Soc Sci Med. 2018;200:114–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Moonen XMH. (H)erkennen en waarderen: Over het (h)erkennen van de noden mensen met licht verstandelijke beperkingen en het bieden van passende ondersteuning. Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors want to thank Evelien van Wingerden for feedback on the Methods section of this article and Jessica Braakman for her comments on the text linked to the mentalization-based treatment.

Status of the trial

Protocol version 1, dated 18 January 2019. After being granted permission by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Amsterdam location VUmc, the first participants were included in September 2018. Currently, August 2019, data collection is completed. The main results are expected to be published at the end of 2019 or the beginning of 2020.

Funding

This research is funded by The Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development ZonMw, Den Haag, the Netherlands (project number 845004001).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SvW, SD, MW, CS, and PSS made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study. SvW and PSS designed the intervention and started the implementation. SD coordinates the recruitment of the participants and data collection during the study. SvW and SD drafted the present manuscript. MW, PSS, and CS provided suggestions for the improvement of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript and agreed both to be personally accountable for the author’s own contributions and to ensure that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work, even ones in which the author was not personally involved, are appropriately investigated and resolved, and the resolution documented in the literature.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paula S. Sterkenburg.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The research was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Amsterdam location VUmc, the Netherlands (METc VUmc 2018.007, NL60353.029.17) and the Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam (VCWE-2017-171). Prior to data collection, informed consent will be obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

In this manuscript, no data of individual persons are used.

Competing interests

Two authors of this article (SvW and PSS) are also authors of the intervention tested in this study. However, there are no financial stakeholders as the game will be available free of charge. The research team commits reporting the trial findings without regard to outcome. The research funder will have no role in the study design, data collection, management, data analysis and interpretation, writing of subsequent reports, or decision to submit reports for publication.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

SPIRIT 2013 Checklist: Recommended items to address in a clinical trial protocol and related documents (DOC 122 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Derks, S., van Wijngaarden, S., Wouda, M. et al. Effectiveness of the serious game ‘You & I’ in changing mentalizing abilities of adults with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities: a parallel superiority randomized controlled trial. Trials 20, 500 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13063-019-3608-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13063-019-3608-9

Keywords